Introduction
There is a considerable literature which has grown up around the theme of the role of emotion regulation in mental health. Gross defined emotion regulation as "the processes by which individuals influence which emotions they have, when they have them, and how they experience and express them"[1]. According to Gratz and Roemer, identifying, understanding and acceptance of emotions, and deployment of efficient emotion regulation strategies, impulse control and continuing the goal directed behaviors in facing intense emotions count as effective emotion regulation [2].
Also, sequential studies posit that emotion dysregulation plays a critical role in the development and maintenance of emotional disorders [3-6], in recent years, researchers have shown an increased interest in the role of emotion dysregulation in obsessive compulsive disorder. OCD patients considered intrusive thoughts as a process that potentially has negative and salient consequences. In order to reduce the distress caused by these thoughts, they put a lot of effort to decrease negative emotions. In a vicious cycle, suppressing intrusive thoughts and avoidance from negative emotions result in less self-confident in toleration and effective emotional management.
So, they are more likely to engage in ineffective strategies such as compulsions, that prevent them from experiencing these painful emotions and prevents new adaptive learning [7]. This model is consistent with conditioning models of obsessive-compulsive disorder which proposed that intrusive thoughts associated with increased anxiety and distress. If the patient is allowed to perform the rituals, the pain and discomfort will decrease almost immediately.
And if the rituals are delayed, the grief and sorrow will last a little longer (spontaneous decline) and when a person refuses to perform rituals, the next step will be less anxiety. However, performing rituals thwart this achievement [8]. Compared to other emotional disorders, although evidence and theories point the important role of emotional dysregulation in obsession, less attention has been paid to this issue. In initial study of emotion regulation and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in non-clinical population, stern et al revealed that poor understanding and fear of negative and positive emotions associated with OC symptom [9]. According to the Fergus and Bardeen examination, expressive suppression, impulse control difficulties, and emotional clarity predict obsessive–compulsive symptom severity [10].
Same result was obtained from Berman et al. [11]. The endophenotype of emotion dysregulation in OCD was examined in two recent studies. Even though, in one examination, [12] it was showed that sibling of OCD patients compared with healthy control had increased activation in dmPFC, fronto-parietal and temporal areas [13], subsequent examination demonstrated that based on f-MRI data emotion regulation is not a strong endophenotype for OCD.
It seems that, in the literature on OCD, the relative importance of emotion regulation is debated and has not dealt with it. Most studies in this field did not consider the comorbid symptoms. So, the current investigation seeks to determine to what extent emotion dysregulation related to OC symptoms, while the role of generalized anxiety symptoms (a disorder that highly associated with emotion dysregulation facets). The second aim of this research is to identify most significant ED factor in high levels of OC symptoms. This study assesses the role of experiential avoidance as dysfunctional emotion regulation strategy that assists in avoiding discomfort emotion, [14]. Perseverative thinking is examined, as well; as it is a form of failure to regulate effective emotion [15,16] and which related to OC distress [17]. This paper has been divided into two parts. The first part deal with assess unique assossiation of emotion dysregulation factors and OC symptoms, in participants with low GAD symptoms, and the second part focus on comparing heightened OC symptoms and heightened GAD symptoms participants on emotion dysregulation factors.
Methods
This correlational research was done within a three-month period in two universities in Tehran, Iran.
Participants
500 students were recruited for this study. 23 people left more than 10% of the questionnaires blank excluded from the study. The sample consist of 477 participants, 53.3% male and were aged between 19 and 49 (mean=22.51, SD=3.16).
Measures
Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS); a 36-item self-report questionnaire that measures emotion dysregulation patterns. Higher scores demonstrate greater problems in regulating emotions. Nonacceptance of emotional responses, difficulty engaging in Goal-directed behavior, impulse control difficulties, lack of emotional awareness, [18] limited access to emotion regulation strategies, and lack of emotional clarity are its six subscales. Items can be responded ranging from 1 to 5, (1: Almost never, 2: Sometimes, 3: About half the time, 4: Most of the time, and 5: Almost always). Psychometric properties of the scale in original study follow: Cronbach'salpha=.93; test-retest reliability=.88. And in the current study follow: Cronbach's alpha for 36 items=.90, and its 5 subscales ranged from .76 to .86[2].
Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ);The PTQ is a 15-item scale that consists three subscales: core characteristics of repetitive thinking (9 items; e.g., “I can’t stop dwelling on them.”), unproductiveness (3 items, e.g., “My thoughts are not much help to me”), and RNT capturing mental capacity (3 items, e.g., “My thoughts take up all my attention”). The score of items can be varied 0 to 4. The original version of PTQ in German population [19,20]. and its Persian version in Iranian sample has good psychometric properties (21). In the current study, PTQ internal consistency was .93.
Acceptance and Action Questionnaire–II (AAQ-II); it is a 10-item measure of psychological inflexibility or experiential avoidance. Students answered the items using a seven-point Likert scale (ranging from 1: Never true to 7: Always true). Higher scores suggest greater levels of experiential avoidance. The mean alpha coefficient of the AAQ-II is .84 and its stability, during 3 and 12 months is .81 and .79, respectively [21]. The participants filled out the measure and its internal consistency was 82..
Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R); Foa et al. provided this scale to assess OCD symptoms severity, in clinical and non-clinical population. This scale covered six subscales (washing, checking, ordering, obsessing, hoarding, and neutralizing) that each has three items that are scored from 0 to 4. Foa et al. based on empirically supported evidences, suggest cutoff score of 21 for the OCI-R total score as an indicator of the likely presence of clinical OC symptoms. Internal consistency of the subscales was ranged from .77 to .88 and its stability was reported .76 [22]. In this study, alpha coefficient was ranged from .6 to .78.
The Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale (GAD-7); The GAD-7 is a short and fast instrument to screen and assess the severity of generalized anxiety symptoms. By a four-point Likert scale (ranging from 0: Not at all to 3: Earlyevery day), it measures how often the responders experience symptoms of GAD, during two last weeks. Spitzer et al., identified a score ≥ 10 as the optimal cut-off point to diagnosis of GAD. Psychometric properties of this instrument in different settings, including English, Spanish, and Persian populations [23] have been revealed. Cronbach's alpha for GAD-7 was high (α=.83), in this research.
Positive and Negative Affect Schedule- negative affect subscale; this subscale is a 10-item, self-report scale for measuring aspects of negative affect [24,25]. Items are graded from 1 to 5. In the initial study has demonstrated that the scale has high internal consistency and appropriate level of stability. In this sample, Cronbach's alpha is calculated [26-29].
Procedures
Students frequently were found in libraries, campus and dormitories of universities of Tehran, Iran. When the participants were invited, the purpose of the research was clearly explained to them. Those who agreed to participate in the study first filled out an informed consent form and then, the six questionnaires. There is a body of research that corroborates the role of emotion dysregulation on GAD and high levels of anxiety [30,31]. Therefore, aiming to test the first hypothesis and in order to minimize the likelihood that heightened anxiety would be a confounding factor in the relationship between OCD symptoms and emotion regulation variables. Data from 357 cases whose scores in GAD-7 were below 10 were analyzed. And for the second hypothesis, two 30-case groups-analog OCD and analog GAD-were formed, by using the cut-off scores of OCI-R and GAD-7. Those participants with high scores and those with low ones in both scale, did not enter the further analysis. The analog OCD group received a score between 30 and 45 in OCI-R and a score between 1 and 6 in GAD-7 questionnaire. On the other hand, the score of the analog GAD group in the OCI-R was between 0 and 20 and in GAD-7 was between 10 and 21. Ethical approval was obtained from Ethical Review Board of University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran [32,33].
Data analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS statistics 22. Multivariate normality was checked with skewness and kurtosis [34,35]. All items in the sample showed normality. In addition to descriptive statistics, Pearson correlation coefficient and hierarchical regression analysis were utilized to examine the relationship between components of emotion dysregulation and OCD symptoms. Last but not least, independent-samples t-tests were conducted to compare repetitive thinking, experiential avoidance and emotion dysregulation in the analog OCD and GAD groups [36].
Results
Descriptive statistics
Descriptive statistics of all variables are showed in (Table 1). All the questionnaires indicated sufficient internal consistency in this research. Except one subscale of DERS, "Lack of emotional awareness" that was omitted from the analysis, due to its weak alpha coefficients (α=.30).
|
Mean |
Standard deviation |
a |
Gad-7 |
4.98 |
2.53 |
0.83 |
Negative affect |
30.13 |
4.62 |
0.61 |
OCI-R |
19.58 |
10.26 |
0.88 |
Checking |
2.89 |
2.11 |
0.6 |
Ordering |
4.3 |
2.68 |
0.78 |
Neutralizing |
1.91 |
2.1 |
0.66 |
Obsessions |
4.1 |
2.45 |
0.76 |
Hoarding |
3.88 |
2.27 |
0.65 |
Washing |
2.51 |
2.36 |
0.72 |
PTQ |
22.87 |
11.12 |
0.93 |
AAQ-II |
31.07 |
9.37 |
0.82 |
Nonacceptance |
13.31 |
5.11 |
0.86 |
Goals |
15.06 |
4.31 |
0.81 |
Impulse |
14.66 |
4.6 |
0.81 |
Strategies |
18.05 |
5.96 |
0.86 |
Clarity |
11.11 |
3.49 |
0.75 |
DERS |
70.06 |
17.98 |
0.9 |
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics
Relations between OC symptoms and aspects of emotion dysregulation
Approximately, all measures (PTQ, AAQ-II and DERS and its subscales) were significantly associated with various symptoms of OC (see Table 2). Most of the correlation coefficients indicate moderate magnitude (range r's=.20 to .54). Negative affect was associated with all OC symptoms, except "neutralizing" and "washing". Repetitive thinking (PTQ) was related to all symptom, also its relation with "obsession" was most strong [37,38]. This condition also applied to experiential avoidance (AAQ-II). Although DERS and total OCI-R have a moderate correlation (r=.38), relationships between their subscales were estimated too weak to moderate. "Limited access to emotion regulation strategies", "nonacceptance of emotional responses and "Impulse control difficulties" were all significantly associated with heightened distress related to OC symptoms. There was no support for a significant correlation between "difficulty engaging in Goal-directed behavior" and "ordering" and "neutralizing" symptoms and between "lack of emotional clarity" and "ordering" are showed in (Table 2)[39,40].
|
OCI-R |
Checking |
Ordering |
Neutralizing |
Obsessions |
Hoarding |
Washing |
Negative Affect |
.23** |
.20** |
.16** |
0.1 |
.23** |
.20** |
0.09 |
PTQ |
.33** |
.21** |
.13* |
.10* |
.54** |
.36** |
.11* |
AAQ-II |
.39** |
.29** |
.17** |
.18** |
.53** |
.37** |
.19* |
Nonacceptance |
33** |
.22** |
.14** |
.20** |
.45** |
.29** |
.15** |
Goals |
.21** |
.11* |
0.07 |
0.01 |
.36** |
.26** |
.12** |
Impulse |
.32** |
.21** |
.11* |
.19** |
.44** |
.29** |
.18** |
Strategies |
.35** |
.26** |
.11* |
.16** |
.50** |
.37** |
.15** |
Clarity |
.25** |
.16** |
0.04 |
.19** |
.33** |
.20** |
.16** |
DERS |
.38** |
.25** |
.12* |
.19** |
.51** |
.36** |
.20** |
Table 2. Correlations among Oc Symptoms and Aspects of Emotion Dysregulation
Note: N=357, *: p< .05, **: p< .01, ***: p< .001
Unique associations
Before data analyzing, multivariate outliers were identified based on Mahalanobis distance. Aligned with the assumption of singularity, no independent variable (IV) associates with others.correlation coefficients ranged from r=.26 to r=.68. Hierarchal regression analyses were employed to test whether each emotion dysregulation variables shared unique variance with OC symptoms, independent of the role of negative affect. In order to test this hypothesis, while total score of OCI-R serving as the criterion variable, negative affect was entered into Stage 1 and PTQ, AAQ-II and the DERS subscales were entered into Stage 2 of a regression model. The outcome of the two models is found in (Table 3). In stage 1, it is indicated that negative affectcontributed significantly to the regression model (F=19.81, p<.001), and accounted for 5% of the variation in OCI-R score. In stage 2, Adding PTQ, AAQ-II and DERS subscales to the regression model explained an 19% of the variation in OCI-R and this change in R² was significant, F=11.41, p<.001. After including all eight IVs in the regression model, negative affect was not a significant predictor OCI-R score. The only predictors of OCI-R score were AAQ-II and "Limited access to emotion regulation strategies" which explained 22% and 15% of the variation of OCI-R score, respectively [40,41].
Variables |
B |
SE (B) |
ß |
B |
SE (B) |
ß |
R |
R2 |
R2 |
Stage 1 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
0.23 |
0.05 |
0.05 |
Negative Affect |
0.51 |
0.11 |
.23*** |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Stage 2 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
0.45 |
0.2 |
0.19 |
Negative Affect |
|
|
|
0.21 |
0.11 |
0.05 |
|
|
|
PTQ |
|
|
|
0.1 |
0.06 |
0.1 |
|
|
|
AAQ-II |
|
|
|
0.24 |
0.07 |
.22** |
|
|
|
Nonacceptance |
|
|
|
0.19 |
0.13 |
0.09 |
|
|
|
Goals |
|
|
|
-0.32 |
0.16 |
-0.13 |
|
|
|
Impulse |
|
|
|
0.33 |
0.16 |
.15* |
|
|
|
Strategies |
|
|
|
0.06 |
0.14 |
0.04 |
|
|
|
Clarity |
|
|
|
0.04 |
0.17 |
0.01 |
|
|
|
Table3. Regression Results Examining Unique Associations between Repetitive Thinking, Experiential Avoidance And Emotion Regulation And Obsessive–Compulsive Symptoms
Note. N= 357 *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p< .001
Comparing levels of emotion dysregulation, the analog OCD group and analog GAD
Scores of analog OCD group and analog GAD group were compared through independent-samples t-tests (Table 4). As a result, analog GAD group had greater difficulties in impulse control [42]. Additionally they reported significantly less access to emotion regulation strategies. Nonetheless, in the others they did not differ from analog OCD group [43,44].
Measures |
Analog OCD (n = 30) |
Analog GAD (n = 30) |
df |
t |
p |
Mean |
SD |
Mean |
SD |
AAQ-II |
35.27 |
7.82 |
34.2 |
9.41 |
58 |
0.48 |
0.63 |
PTQ |
25.84 |
9.26 |
28.59 |
9.62 |
58 |
-1.12 |
0.26 |
Nonacceptance |
15.67 |
4.92 |
15.75 |
5.09 |
58 |
-0.05 |
0.95 |
Goals |
15.06 |
4.2 |
17.66 |
4.15 |
58 |
-2.4 |
0.01 |
Impulse |
15.84 |
4.67 |
16.8 |
3.87 |
58 |
-0.85 |
0.39 |
Strategies |
19.24 |
6.25 |
22.88 |
4.81 |
58 |
-2.52 |
0.01 |
Clarity |
11.56 |
2.21 |
11.92 |
3.22 |
51.37 |
-0.49 |
0.61 |
DERS |
95.48 |
16.67 |
101.82 |
13.96 |
58 |
-1.59 |
0.11 |
Table 4. Comparison of Mean Scores on Emotion Dysregulation Measures
Discussion
The main purpose of this paper is to clarify the relationship between emotion dysregulation aspects and OC symptom. In accordance with previous studies, the outcomes of this investigation evidence that deficiency in effective emotion regulation associated with OC symptoms. Obsession had moderate and the strongest correlation with repetitive thinking, experiential avoidance and five aspects of difficulties in emotion regulation [45,46]. But other symptoms weakly to moderately correlated with emotion dysregulation factors. This result is consistent with those of Fergus and Bardeen and Stern et al.. Furthermore, it accords with observations of which demonstrated reducing repetitive thinking and suppression accompanied reducing OC symptoms [47].
The most interesting finding was that approximately emotion dysregulator is not a strong predict OC symptoms severity. Among the five aspects of difficulties in emotion regulation, impulse control difficulties alongside experiential avoidance, was the poor predictor of OC severity. These results matched those observed in Fergus and Bardeen study; except that they found emotional clarity as a facet of emotion regulation that shared unique associations with OC symptoms [48].
It seems possible that these results are due to the excluding participants with high scores in GAD from analyzing. More than any other psychiatric disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder is most commonly associated with anxiety disorders, including GAD [49,50]. And numerous studies highlight the role of emotional dysregulation in pathological worry and generalized anxiety disorder. Thus, the observation of emotional dysregulation in obsessive-compulsive patients may be explained by the presence of accompanying high levels of pathological anxiety, not by OC symptoms. Further studies, especially experimental research that comprise OCD patients and control group can confirm or decline this claim [51].
Not only anxiety disorders, but also other psychiatric disorders that are highly comorbid with OCD, somehow related to emotional dysregulation. Major depressive disorder, bipolar mood disorders, trauma- related disorders, somatic symptoms, substance use disorders are among the ones that correlated with OCD [52,53]. More studies are demanded to reveal the part of comorbid condition in relationship between OCD and emotion dysregulation [54].
Although these findings are in agreement with Thorsen et al. finding which showed emotion regulation does not represent a strong endophenotype in obsessive – compulsive disorder, they differ from those that showed emotion regulation difficulties had a salient role in OC severity [55,56].
These results may assist us in understanding how emotional, cognitive or behavior avoidance can effect on and maintenance OC symptoms, even in nonclinical population. In accordance with conditional model of OCD, in confronting an OC situation, a patient tends to avoid their emotions by resortingto an impulsive behavior. Consequently, they are not exposed to their emotion and do not learn how to tolerate or reduce the negative emotion efficiently, even if the person has access to emotion regulation strategies. It can be deduced from these results that maybe because of other factors such as intolerance of ambiguity and intolerance of distress, compulsive behaviors are the first and only choice [57,58]. This speculation would be truer for washing, checking, hoarding and obsession symptoms rather than for neutralizing and ordering. Contrariwise, it is possible these symptoms serve personal goals [59].
The last finding suggested that in congruence with transdiagnostic postulation, emotion regulation is a common factor among GAD and OCD. State that people with generalized anxiety disorder use Anxiety and worry to avoid emotional processing; so, emotion regulation skills deficits would be known as the main problems occur in these patients [60]. In the same way, compulsive behaviors prevent emotional processing. Nevertheless, it seems that GAD patients had greater difficulties in engaging in goal-directed behaviors and approaching functional emotion regulation strategies. These differences can be explained in part by the nature of psychopathology of GAD and OCD. Nearly continues worry and physiological symptoms interferes with daily function and Goal-directed behaviors. People who cannot effectively manage and regulate emotional responses to daily events, experience longer and more intense periods of distress and may turn into significant anxiety and depression disorders. This outcome has not previously been described, but is consistent with Davoodi et al. work. Due to these results came from the non-clinical population, we must be cautious in our conclusions.
Conclusion
This project was undertaken to examine the role of emotion dysregulation in obsessive – compulsive symptoms severity. The results contribute to our comprehending of the conceptualization of OCD based on emotion regulation theories. In summary, this research is not contrary to emotion regulation models. Still, other factors can take precedence over emotion regulation in psychopathology of OCD, for instance cognitive distortions, behavioral avoidance and metacognition factors (thought control, thought fusion, etc.). As CBT, ERP and MCT are the empirical supported treatments for OCD. At last, presumably between the range of mild to moderate conditions of OC symptoms, emotion dysregulation have less weight, and maybe suppression and avoidance in this condition do not interfere with person's function, whereas in moderate to severe form, serious emotion dysregulation may lead to higher levels of the disorder, anxiety and depression comorbidities, more repetitive and intrusive thinking that suppression is no longer useful. Dimensional models of the latent structure of all OC symptoms, may consist with this conclusion. This conclusion will be of interest to the researchers working on OCD psychopathology and interventions.
The presented paper has also faced some limitations. The major limitation of the present investigation is using limited questionnaires, which does not allow for a precise and comprehensive conclusion. Sampling from students makes these findings less generalizable.
Several questions still remain to be answered: Weather OCD patients with and without emotional disorders comorbidities differs in emotion regulation function and weather emotional factors like emotional schemas, including emotional inhibition explained OC symptoms. Considerably, more work will need to determine emotion dysregulation factors in subgroups of OCD. Examining effectiveness of emotion regulation techniques and interventions and comparing them with evidence – based interventions for OCD, would be fruitful area for future works.
Highlights
• Emotion regulation is a common factor among OCD and GAD
• Emotion dysregulation is not a strong predict OC symptoms severity unlike GAD
• Some aspects of emotion regulation such as impulse control difficulties and experiential avoidance predict the severity of OCD.
Plain Language Summery
Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a common and debilitating disorder. One of the factors that correlate with OCD is emotion dysregulation.It is important to identify the aspects of emotional regulation and the relationship between the aspects with obsessive-compulsive disorder, which can be used to treat and prevent OCD.Given that generalized anxiety disorder is often accompanies obsessive-compulsive disorder, we want to see which aspects of emotional dysregulation are most closely associated with generalized anxiety symptoms and which ones are associated with OD symptoms.
Ethical Considerations
Compliance with ethical guidelines
This study was approved by the research ethics committee of the University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran (IR.USWR.REC.1395.164). To follow the ethics, consent forms were obtained from all participants and only individuals who were desired were enrolled in the study. It was explained to all participants that the intervention is due to a clinical research project.
Funding
This study was supported as a research project by Psychosis Research Center in University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences (grant number: 1202).
Author’s contribution
F.M and M.K designed the first framework and analyzed the data. M.K carried out the implementation. F.M. conducted the calculations. F.M and M.K wrote the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest.
Acknowledgements
We would like to express our gratitude to the respected individuals who have cooperated in the implementation of this study.
References
- Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. review of general psychology. 1998;2(3):271-299.
- Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2004; 26(1):41-54.
- Garke MÅ, Isacsson NH, Sörman K, Bjureberg J, Hellner C, Gratz KL, et al. Emotion dysregulation across levels of substance use. Psychiatry Research. 2021;296:113662.
- Vanderlind WM, Millgram Y, Baskin-Sommers AR, Clark MS, Joormann J. Understanding positive emotion deficits in depression: From emotion preferences to emotion regulation. Clinical Psychology Review. 2020;76:101826.
- Dryman MT, Heimberg RG. Emotion regulation in social anxiety and depression: a systematic review of expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Clinical Psychology Review. 2018;65:17-42.
- Prefit A-B, Cândea DM, Szentagotai-Tatar A. Emotion regulation across eating pathology: A meta-analysis. Appetite. 2019;143:104438.
- Calkins AW, Berman NC,Wilhelm S. Recent advances in research on cognition and emotion in OCD: a review. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2013;15(5):357.
- Taylor S, Abramowitz J, McKay D. Cognitive-behavioral models of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological treatment of OCD: Fundamentals and beyond. 2006;9-29.
- Stern MR, Nota JA, Heimberg RG, Holaway RM, Coles ME. An initial examination of emotion regulation and obsessive compulsive symptoms. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders. 2014;3(2):109-14.
- Fergus TA, Bardeen JR. Emotion regulation and obsessive–compulsive symptoms: A further examination of associations. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders. 2014;3(3):243-8.
- Berman NC, Shaw AM, Curley EE, Wilhelm S. Emotion regulation and obsessive-compulsive phenomena in youth. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders. 2018;19:44-9.
- Thorsen AL, de Wit SJ, de Vries FE, Cath DC, Veltman DJ, van der Werf YD, et al. THE endophenotype of emotional regulation in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (OCD). European Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;27(6):619.
- Thorsen AL, de Wit SJ, de Vries FE, Cath DC, Veltman DJ, van der Werf YD, et al. Emotion regulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder, unaffected siblings, and unrelated healthy control participants. biological psychiatry. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2019;4(4):352-60.
- Kashdan TB, Barrios V, Forsyth JP, Steger MF. Experiential avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: comparisons with coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44(9):1301-1320.
- Magee JC, Harden KP, Teachman BA. Psychopathology and thought suppression: a quantitative review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2012;32(3):189-201.
- Langen M, Durston S, Kas MJ, van Engeland H, Staal WG. The neurobiology of repetitive behavior:… and men. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2011;35(3):356-65.
- Lustberg D, Iannitelli AF, Tillage RP, Pruitt M, Liles LC, Weinshenker D. Central norepinephrine transmission is required for stress-induced repetitive behavior in two rodent models of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychopharmacology. 2020;237(7):1973-87.
- Santore LA, Gerber A, Gioia AN, Bianchi R, Talledo F, Peris TS, et al. Felt but not seen: Observed restricted repetitive behaviors are associated with self-report—but not parent-report—obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2020;24(4):983-94.
- Ehring T, Zetsche U, Weidacker K, Wahl K, Schönfeld S, Ehlers A. The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2011;42(2):225-32.
- Ehring T, Raes F, Weidacker K, Emmelkamp P. Validation of the Dutch Version of the Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ-NL). European Journal of Psychological Assessment-Eur J Psychol Assess. 2011;28:102-8.
- Kami M, Moloodi R, Mazidi M, Ehring T, Mansoori AK, Nodooshan MB, et al. Measuring repetitive thinking in Iran: Psychometric properties of Persian version of Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences. 2019;148:101-109.
- Bond FW, Hayes SC, Baer RA, Carpenter KM, Guenole N, Orcutt HK, et al. Preliminary psychometric properties of the acceptance and action questionnaire–II: A revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behavior Therapy. 2011;42(4):676-88.
- Foa EB, Huppert JD, Leiberg S, Langner R, Kichic R, Hajcak G, et al. The obsessive-compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychol Assess. 2002;14(4):485-96.
- Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(10):1092-1097.
- Mills SD, Fox RS, Malcarne VL, Roesch SC, Champagne BR, Sadler GR. The psychometric properties of the generalized anxiety disorder-7 scale in Hispanic Americans with English or Spanish language preference. Cultur Divers Ethnic Minor Psychol. 2014;20(3):463-468.
- Omani-Samani R, Maroufizadeh S, Ghaheri A, Navid B. Generalized anxiety Disorder-7 (GAD-7) in people with infertility: a reliability and validity study. Middle east fertility society journal. 2018;23(4):446-9.
- Naeinian M, SHAIRI M, Sharifi M, Hadian M. To study reliability and validity for a brief measure for assessing Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD-7). 2011.
- Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063-1070.
- Mennin DS, Fresco DM, O'Toole MS, Heimberg RG. A randomized controlled trial of emotion regulation therapy for generalized anxiety disorder with and without co-occurring depression. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 2018;86(3):268-281.
- Ouellet C, Langlois F, Provencher M, Gosselin P. Intolerance of uncertainty and difficulties in emotion regulation: Proposal for an integrative model of generalized anxiety disorder. European Review of Applied Psychology. 2019;69(1):9-18.
- Renna ME, Seeley SH, Heimberg RG, Etkin A, Fresco DM, Mennin DS. Increased attention regulation from emotion regulation therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2018;42(2):121-134.
- Cisler JM, Olatunji BO. Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. Current psychiatry reports. 2012;14(3):182-7.
- Wei MA, Van Kirk N, Reid AM, Garner LE, Krompinger JW, Crosby JM, et al. Emotion regulation strategy use and symptom change during intensive treatment of transitional age youth patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapy. 2020;30(2):95-102.
- Menzies RE, Zuccala M, Sharpe L, Dar-Nimrod I. Are anxiety disorders a pathway to obsessive-compulsive disorder? Different trajectories of OCD and the role of death anxiety. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry. 2020; 75(3): 1-6.
- Hedman E, Ljótsson B, Axelsson E, Andersson G, Rück C, Andersson E. Health anxiety in obsessive compulsive disorder and obsessive compulsive symptoms in severe health anxiety: An investigation of symptom profiles. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2017;45:80-86.
- Pallanti S, Grassi G, Sarrecchia ED, Cantisani A, Pellegrini M. Obsessive-compulsive disorder comorbidity: clinical assessment and therapeutic implications. Front Psychiatry. 2011;2:70.
- Ranney RM, Cox CM, Behar E. Relationships between emotion regulation and depression in high and low worriers. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2020;42(1):101-110.
- Bosley HG, Sandel DB, Fisher AJ. Idiographic dynamics of positive affect in GAD: Modeling emotion regulation at the person level. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 2020;36(3):500.
- Loevaas MES, Sund AM, Patras J, Martinsen K, Hjemdal O, Neumer SP, et al. Emotion regulation and its relation to symptoms of anxiety and depression in children aged 8–12 years: does parental gender play a differentiating role. BMC Psychology. 2018;6(1):42.
- McLaughlin KA, Mennin DS, Farach FJ. The contributory role of worry in emotion generation and dysregulation in generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45(8):1735-1752.
- Suveg C, Morelen D, Brewer GA & Thomassin K. The Emotion Dysregulation Model of Anxiety: A preliminary path analytic examination. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2010;24(8):924-930.
- Jones PJ, Mair P, Riemann BC, Mugno BL, McNally RJ. A network perspective on comorbid depression in adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of anxiety disorders. 2018;53:1-8.
- Mucci F, Toni C, Favaretto E, Vannucchi G, Marazziti D, Perugi G. Obsessive-compulsive disorder with comorbid bipolar disorders: clinical features and treatment implications. Current Medicinal Chemistry. 2018;25(41):5722-30.
- Van Kirk N, Fletcher TL, Wanner JL, Hundt N, Teng EJ. Implications of comorbid OCD on PTSD treatment: A case study. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic. 2018;82(4):344-59.
- Aksu MH, Sahiner SY, Sahiner IV, Koparal B, Utku C, Cosar B, et al. Relationship between somatization and psychiatric symptoms, especially anxiety, depression, alexithymia, and severity of addiction in male patients with alcohol and heroin addiction. Dusunen Adam. 2020;33(2):120-9.
- Ecker AH, Stanley MA, Smith TL, Teng EJ, Fletcher TL, Van Kirk N, et al. Co-occurrence of obsessive-compulsive disorder and substance use disorders among US veterans: prevalence and mental health utilization. Journal of cognitive psychotherapy. 2019;33(1):23-32.
- ASLI AZAD M, MANSHAEE G, GHAMARANI A. Effectiveness of acceptance and commitment therapy on cognitive emotion regulation and intolerance of uncertainty of the students with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychology of Exceptional Individuals. 2020;9(36): 001035.
- Shameli l, Mehrabizadeh Honarmand M, Naami A, Davodi I. The effectiveness of emotion-focused therapy on emotion regulation styles and severity of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in women with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology. 2019; 24(4): 00469.
- Taylor S, Abramowitz J, McKay D. Cognitive-behavioral models of obsessive-compulsive disorder. 2006; 9-29.
- Tolin DF, Abramowitz JS, Brigidi BD, Foa EB. Intolerance of uncertainty in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of anxiety disorders. 2003;17(2):233-242.
- Gillett CB, Bilek EL, Hanna GL, Fitzgerald KD. Intolerance of uncertainty in youth with obsessive-compulsive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder: A transdiagnostic construct with implications for phenomenology and treatment. Clinical psychology review. 2018;60:100-108.
- Mennin DS, Heimberg RG, Turk CL, Fresco DM. Preliminary evidence for an emotion dysregulation model of generalized anxiety disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2005;43(10):1281-1310.
- Hezel DM, Simpson HB. Exposure and response prevention for obsessive-compulsive disorder: A review and new directions. Indian J Psychiatry. 2019;61(1):S85-S92.
- Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®): American Psychiatric Pub. 2013.
- Vedala R, Macaluso MM. When worry is excessive: Easing the burden of GAD. The Journal of Family Practice. 2020;69(7): 357-361.
- Osborne TL, Michonski J, Sayrs J, Welch SS, Anderson LK. Factor structure of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) in adult outpatients receiving Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2017;39(2):355-71.
- Afshari B, Hasani J. Study of dialectical behavior therapy versus cognitive behavior therapy on emotion regulation and mindfulness in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy. 2020;50(4):305-312.
- Davoodi A, Neshat Doost HT, Abdi MR, Talebi H. Comparison of cognitive emotion regulation strategies and understanding emotions in obsessive compulsive, generalized anxiety, and social anxiety disorders. Knowledge & Research in Applied Psychology. 2014;15(58):69-78.
- Leeuwerik T, Cavanagh K, Strauss C. Patient adherence to cognitive behavioural therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of anxiety disorders. 2019;68:102135.
- Foa EB. Cognitive behavioral therapy of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2010;12(2):199-207.